random thoughts to oil the mind

Category: Books Page 5 of 14

[:en]Book reviews amongst other things.[:de]Buchrezensionen unter anderem

The Dinner

The DinnerTold through the eyes of a decidedly unreliable narrator, The Dinner is an elegantly simple novel served up over five courses. From the casual opening aperitif to the grittier, questionable digestif, the book layers on the story over a meal in a swanky restaurant.

The story revolves around two families who meet to discuss a problem with their children. The two men are brothers: the one being our narrator, who seems to have little time for the second, who we soon learn is a high-flying politician likely to become the next prime minister. During the course of the meal, details are filtered in, largely via flashbacks, as we learn the nature of the ‘problem’ the couples are having with their children, as well as becoming increasingly suspicious about the motives of our initially amiable narrator. There are plenty of shocks and twists to get the book through to its conclusion, and leaves the reader posed with the question on the book’s cover: just how far would you go to protect the ones you love?

The writing style is very colloquial, each course easily digested, with the whole meal edible in one sitting. At times humourous, occasionally shocking, there is an interesting moral question being posed, but I think the book would like to be taken more seriously than it deserves. For me, it was an enjoyable read, but nothing more than entertainment.

Still, it remains a great little tale, light bite size portions gradually building up a satisfying meal, which depending on your palate might leave you feeling a little queasy – and nevertheless thanking the chef for it.

The Ministry of Fear: An Entertainment

Ministry of FearGraham Greene originally divided his works into novels and ‘entertainments’, separating his popular work from those he wished his literary career to be remembered for. In later life, this distinction would be blurred until it was dropped entirely. The Ministry of Fear is one of these earlier works labelled an entertainment. Made a year later into a film directed by Fritz Lang, it was written in the middle of wartime, and on the surface is a typical espionage thriller in the tradition of John Buchan’s The Thirty-Nine Steps, dealing with a Nazi spy ring operating in London during the blitz. On its own, the plot is gripping enough to carry the book through to the end, and those bits we can regard as ‘entertainment’ made their way into the film.

But as with many of Greene’s works, it’s the inner conflict which is missing from the silver screen translation. We learn early on that the main protagonist is racked by guilt over the murder–what we would today most likely see only as a mercy killing–of his wife. This concentration on the individual, amid the scaled chaos of the blitz, makes this short novel so interesting. Much of it seems quite dated now, but there is still plenty of relevance in a society trying to come to terms with the issue of euthanasia.

Aside from the juxtaposition of a thrilling little spy plot and the psychological reflections, this short book is also an advert for Greene’s art. The writing is simply superb, an absolute pleasure to read, full of inventiveness without the overt self-conceit of trying too hard. Another reviewer pointed out that this short novel took longer to read than he had imagined. I’d suggest that comes as a result of needing to read every word and understand it, not skim over lines of trite, repetitive text as in many other novels. To skim would be to rob oneself of most of the pleasure.

For me, Graham Greene remains the greatest English language novelist never to have won the Nobel Prize. As an entertainment, rather than a novel, The Ministry of Fear lends itself as an excellent introduction to his greater literature.

Atlas Shrugged

Atlas ShruggedPerhaps the most significant book in post-war American literature, one which has regained popularity since the start of the economic crisis, Altas Shrugged is the embodiment of an ideal society, the ultimate vehicle for Ayn Rand’s philosophy of objectivism. Weighing in at over 1,000 pages of tightly-packed print, it’s also one of the longest novels in English literature. Is it any good?

Well, as a novel, Atlas Shrugged unfortunately falls flat, in ways that Rand’s first novel, We the Living, didn’t. There is foremost no humanity in the novel, the characters are dismembered, dessicated mouthpieces to Rand’s philosophical diatribes, with everyone fitting neatly into ‘good’ and ‘bad’ camps. Rand herself claimed that using characters as symbols was never her intention: “My characters are persons in whom certain human attributes are focused more sharply and consistently than in average human beings.” But what we are left with are flimsy apparitions, lobotomised automatons fulfilling the roles required of them to extol the virtues of her philosophy. Even this is taken to extremes, with one of the proponents delivering a 60-page long theoretical speech around which the rest of the novel might well be seen as scaffolding.

To complement this set of lifeless characters is a plot which similarly confounds understanding. In an America which technologically resembles the period in which Rand was writing, yet industrially feels set in an earlier period, and borrows heavily from the Great Depression, the main events and the decisions of the characters jar heavily with what the reader knows and expects from society. As another reviewer pointed out, what’s missing is the overt understanding that the story takes place in a parallel world or a different timeframe, to create a genuine sense of credibility. True, there are some hints that push this novel into the realms of science fiction–a super metal alloy, power derived from static electricity, weapons based on sound waves etc.–but the world is definitely our own, even if the people and their decisions are alien. Key to the story is the gradual collapse of the economic system, and the disappearance of the champions of industry. What happens in Rand’s universe when the creative minds of the world go on strike? Apparently, they settle down on the frontier and, working one month a year, create a fully-fledged miniature utopia. Personally, I imagine they’d starve.

A bad book can still be a good delivery vehicle for an interesting message. Yet this unwieldy book fails even to achieve the latter. For its mammoth length, Rand’s message could have been relatively concise, but for the plot’s repetitiveness. If you are interested in Rand’s philosophy, there are plenty of other places to turn which will provide a far more succinct and detailed explanation, without the repetition or padding necessary for its delivery in novel form. Whether you find place for Rand’s philosophy in your own, or like Gore Vidal consider it “nearly perfect in its immorality”, there are simply better summaries available. For the converted, this is probably a wonderful book, but for anyone else it simply isn’t worth risking the investment of time and energy.

No one can deny this book’s enduring popularity. That alone gives rise to curiosity strong enough to keep it fresh in the public consciousness. But it is a far cry from a great piece of literature, and as an allegory, a philosophical harbinger, its ponderous and verbose nature should have the curious turn elsewhere. The novel opens with the question: “Who is John Galt?” A thousand pages of largely disappointing text will reveal the answer, but you’d be better served just reading the appendix.

Thinking, Fast and Slow

Thinking, Fast and SlowThis post is also available in German.

The qualities of human nature described in “Thinking, fast and slow” are already known to all of us at some level. We are all taught in society to believe in the rational, thinking human being as distinct from his animal peers, yet at this book excellently details, there are plenty of ways in which our seemingly rational decisions can be bent and perverted by various forms of bias. Daniel Kahneman details these two seemingly incongruous facets of our nature as two ‘distinct’ halves: System 1 represents our autonomous, unthinking, reflex and subconscious reactions, whereas System 2 is that logical, calculating being we consider ourselves to be. Much of the relevant research covered in this volume was pioneered and conducted by Kahneman and his late colleague Amos Tversky, to whom this volume is dedicated.

At root, the interplay between System 1 and System 2 rests upon the fact that we are naturally adapted to choose the path of least resistance, i.e. we make decisions which require the least amount of effort. Whilst this does not necessarily mean that we (or our System 2s) are not making the decisions, it does sometimes result in our System 2s acting merely as auditors of the information being passed on by System 1. If that information appears to fit the facts, it is taken at face value, unchanged and unedited. As a result, this ‘quick thinking’ leads to errors and biases of which we are almost entirely unaware.

As a summary of decades of research, the book deals with a lot of extremely interesting aspects of these decision-making processes. Each of these is handled in turn and alone, although many of them are linked and could in some ways a different impressions of the same phenomenon. For example, an issue known as ‘anchoring’ is investigated, a truly staggering anomaly in which a decision can be influenced by an entirely unrelated and random suggestion placed before us: Kahneman provides us with the example of a set of experienced judges whose sentence decisions were seen to be tilted by the results of a dice roll.

There is a lot of ground to summarise within these pages, and Kahneman does an excellent job of presenting some fairly mundane experimental data in a way in which it becomes clear to the layman, how insightful and potent the results truly are. The first half of the book in particular is an extremely fluid read, the experimental data plays second fiddle to clear evaluations of both experiments and their results. Whilst some aspects are dealt with purely theoretically, others are highlighted in terms of their effect on certain people in society, and Kahneman makes no bones about pointing out the absurd decisions of stock brokers, businessmen, or even his own psychology students. Another nice feature of the book is that many of the chapters start with a little test which readers themselves can do, becoming a part of the experiments, easily the best way to highlight precisely how ‘un-rational’ our minds can truly function.

The overarching irony of this book is that it seems to want to prove one of the theories explored between its very covers, that of our ‘experiencing’ and ‘remembering’ selves. The evidence suggests that even if the vast majority of an experience was born with enjoyment, if the end was tainted, our memory of the positive experience will be overridden by the negative. Unfortunately, this book is guilty of the very same: it opens beautifully with some lucid and unhampered prose, plenty of example tests and real world extrapolations, occasional anecdotes and witty asides. Yet the latter half of the book feels like it was written by a different Kahneman or for a different reader; it is turgid, almost lethargic, sticky with academic language, no longer peppered with as many human insights, and devoid of example tests for the reader to take part in.

Despite this impression of it being a book of two halves, it is nevertheless highly recommendable to anyone with even a passing interest in psychology or the human mind. One needn’t take away any lessons from the book’s insights, but it would still be nice to think that by giving this book five stars, I’m successfully overcoming the biassed suggestions of System 1 and my ‘remembering self’, and basing my judgement on the rational observations of System 2 and my ‘experiencing self’. Or perhaps I’m being swayed by some anchoring I’m still unaware of…

Thinking, Fast and Slow

Thinking, Fast and SlowDieser Eintrag ist auch auf Englisch verfügbar.

Auf irgendeiner Ebene sind uns die Eigenschaften der menschlichen Natur, die „Thinking, fast and slow“ erläutert, schon bekannt. In unserer Gesellschaft wird die Vernunft und die rationalen Handlungen der Menschen gerne hervorgehoben, Attribute, durch die wir uns sehr gerne gegenüber unseren sonstigen Verwandten im Tierreich einen besonderen Standesdünkel gönnen. Doch wie in diesem Buch so meisterhaft dargestellt, gibt es eine beträchtliche Menge an Faktoren, die unsere scheinbar so vernünftigen Entscheidungen verdrehen und beeinflussen können. Laut Kahneman kann man diese zwei so unvereinbaren Aspekte unseres Wesens als getrennte Systeme betrachten: Das erste System verkörpert unsere gedankenlosen, selbstständigen, unterbewussten und reflexartigen Reaktionen, wogegen das zweite System das logisch handelnde und vernünftige Dasein vertritt, wofür wir uns Menschen eigentlich halten. Viele der Forschungsarbeiten und Ergebnisse, die in diesem Buch behandelt werden, hat Kahneman zusammen mit seinem verstorbenen Kollegen Amos Tversky selbst durchgeführt. Ihm ist das Buch auch gewidmet.

Im Kern, so Kahneman, lässt sich das Wechselspiel zwischen den beiden Systemen auf die Tatsache zurückführen, dass wir von Natur aus immer den Weg des geringsten Widerstandes wählen. Wir treffen nämlich lieber die Entscheidungen, die uns die wenigsten Mühen kosten. Natürlich soll das längst nicht bedeuten, dass wir (bzw. das uns vertretende System 2) die Entscheidungen überhaupt nicht treffen, doch laut Kahneman fungiert das zweite System lediglich als Kontrollkörper der Informationen, die vom ersten System weitergeleitet werden. Wenn diese Daten allem Anschein nach den Tatschen entsprechen, werden sie vom zweiten System ungeprüft und unverändert für bare Münze genommen. Daraus ergibt sich das im Titel erwähnte „schnelle Denken“, das zu Fehlern und Vorurteilen führt, ohne sie zu ahnen.

In dieser Zusammenfassung der Forschungsergebnissen mehrerer Jahrzehnten findet man viele interessante Einsichten in die Vorgänge des Hirns bezüglich der Entscheidungsfindung. Jeden Aspekt handelt Kahneman einzeln und alleine ab, auch wenn sie selbstverständlich nicht alleine agieren und in manchen Fällen sogar eine Verkörperung desselben Phänomens bildet. Zum Beispiel erörtert er die sogenannte Ankerheuristik, eine verblüffende Erscheinung, wodurch wir eine Entscheidung von einer völlig unzusammenhängenden und willkürlichen Anregung beeinflussen lassen, ohne sich derer bewusst zu sein. In dem gelieferten Beispiel zeigt sich, dass die Gerichtsurteile erfahrener Richter sich anscheinend durch einen vollkommen belanglosen Würfelwurf lenken ließen.

In diesen Seiten will Kahneman viele Themen abhandeln, doch seine Stärke liegt vor allem darin, dass er sonst ziemlich banale Versuchsergebnisse in eine sehr begreifliche Art und Weise präsentiert, damit jeder verstehen kann, wie aufschlussreich sie in Wirklichkeit sind, und welche große Schlussfolgerungen, sich daraus ziehen lassen. Vor allem die erste Hälfte des Buches lässt sich sehr leicht lesen, wo die rohen Daten den Experimenten und ihren Auswertungen nur eine unterstützende Rolle spielen. Während manche Themen rein theoretisch behandelt werden, hebt Kahneman andere wegen ihres Einflusses auf bestimmte Schichten der Gesellschaft hervor. Hier macht er auch keinen Hehl daraus, Börsenmakler, Geschäftsmänner oder seine eigenen Psychologiestudenten wegen ihrer absurden Entscheidungen aufzuziehen. Viele Kapitel werden durch einen kleinen Test eingeführt, dem sich der Leser selbst unterziehen kann, um selbst ein Teil des Experiments zu werden. So billig das auch klingen mag, eignen sich diese kleinen Tests hervorragend, die unvernünftigen Arbeitsweisen unseres Hirns zu beweisen und sie dem Leser handgreiflich vor Augen zu führen.

Für mich besteht die große Ironie dieses Buches darin, das es anscheinend eine Theorie beweisen will, die die eigenen Seiten erläutern, nämlich den Unterschied zwischen dem „erfahrenden“ und dem „erinnernden“ Geist. Den experimentellen Ergebnissen zufolge hinterlässt ein mehrheitlich angenehmes Erlebnis eine schlechte Erinnerung, wenn es durch unschönen abschließenden Augenblick verdorben wird, denn in unserem Gedächtnis wird das positive Empfinden durch das ausgangs negative Erlebnis überschrieben. Nach meinem Empfinden leidet dieses Buch leider unter genau dieser Erscheinung. Zu Beginn weist es einen prosaischen und erhellenden Schreibstil auf, gewürzt durch gelegentliche Anekdoten und geistreiche Abschweifungen. Doch die zweite Hälfte kommt einem vor, als wäre sie von einem anderen Kahneman oder für einen anderen Leser verfasst. Besät mit akademischen Begrifflichkeiten ist der Stil plötzlich schwülstig und schwerfällig, und die menschlichen Einsichten sowie Testbeispiele fallen gänzlich weg.

Trotz der zwei unterschiedlichen Hälften kann ich das Buch dennoch herzlich jedem empfehlen, der sich selbst nur flüchtig für die Psychologie oder die kognitiven Wissenschaften interessiert. Den erleuchtenden Erkenntnissen in diesem Buch muss man nichts entnehmen, doch ich schmeichele mir gern, dass ich die parteiischen Vorurteile und Anregungen meines ersten Systems durch die vernünftigen und nüchternen Eindrücke des zweiten überstimme, indem ich das Buch mit fünf Sternen bewerte. Oder lasse ich mich von einem mir noch schleierhaften Anker etwa lenken …

Page 5 of 14

Powered by WordPress & Theme by Anders Norén